TED

転ばぬ先の杖があれば怖いものなし!―楽観主義バイアス(TED)

楽観

ライフハックとしてではなく、英語学習にも極めて有用なのが、著名人が10分程度のプレゼンを行うTEDです。

TED Talksとは、あらゆる分野のエキスパートたちによるプレゼンテーションを無料で視聴できる動画配信サービスのことです。10年ほど前にサービスが開始されてから、政治、心理学、経済、日常生活などの幅広いコンテンツが視聴できることから人気を集めています。

RareJob English Lab

TEDは4000を超える膨大な数の動画があります。しかし慣れないうちは、動画の探し方や視聴のコツが分かりませんよね。この記事では、数多くのTEDを見てきた管理人(塩@saltandshio)が、心を揺さぶられたトークをあらすじと一緒にご紹介します。

ビジネス英会話を効率よく身につけたい方におすすめスクール

シェーン英会話
シェーンは1977年の創業以来、ネイティブ講師が英語を英語で教える「直接教授法」を採用しています。首都圏におけるスクール拠点数は、ネイティブ講師の英会話スクールでNo.1。駅から近いスクールが多いので通いやすく時間を有効に使えます。

スピークバディ パーソナルコーチング
1日1時間の短期集中トレーニングで、あなたの英語力向上をコーチが全力でサポートします。あなたの英語の世界が、劇的に変わります。

ターリ・シャーロット: 楽観主義バイアス

ターリ・シャーロット: 楽観主義バイアス

人間は生まれつき現実的ではなく楽観的なのでしょうか?ターリ・シャーロットは、新しい研究結果により、人間の脳が明るい面を見るように作られていることを示唆し、これが危険であると同時に利点でもあることを説明します(約18分)。

[PR]無料体験レッスン実施中!全国208校、創業40年の老舗英会話スクール【シェーン英会話】

『自分はそうならない』という楽観主義

あなたは、自分がガンになる確率はどれくらいだと思いますか?また、交通事故に遭う確率や離婚率はどれくらいだと思いますか。じつは、私たちはこれらのことが自分にも起きるかもしれないという確率を過小評価する傾向にあります。

一方、自分がどれほど健康的で長生きするか、または仕事上で成功する可能性がどれくらいあるかということについては、過大評価しがちです。これらをターリ・シャーロットは『楽観主義バイアス』と呼んでいます。

結婚を例にとると、西洋における離婚率は約40%といわれています。しかし、このような統計が出ているにもかかわらず、離婚専門の弁護士でさえ自分たちが離婚することになる確率をかなり過小評価しています。これが楽観主義バイアスです

ほかにも、自分の子供には才能があると思っていたり、ほかの家庭はどうであろうと自分たちの暮らしは良くなると考えている人はたくさんいます。ですが、隣に座っている人や、国や世界の将来に関してはどちらかというと悲観的に見ています。

にもかかわらず、自分自身の将来に対する楽観主義は根強く揺らぎません。

それも、魔法によって良い結果がもたらせられると思っている訳ではなく、自分たちにその結果を生み出す能力が備わっていると思っているのです。

But private optimism about our own personal future remains persistent.

And it doesn’t mean that we think things will magically turn out okay, but rather that we have the unique ability to make it so.

これが、『自分はそうならない』という楽観主義です。

[PR]まずは無料カウンセリング”続けるため”の オンライン英語コーチ「スピークバディ パーソナルコーチング」

幸福の秘訣は『低い期待を持つこと』ではない3つの理由

『自分はそうならない』という楽観主義によって、多くの人は様々なシチュエーションで一度は痛い目をみます。学校のテストがその良い例かもしれませんね。すると、その痛い目を見た人の中から、今度は『幸福の秘訣は低い期待を持つことだ』と言い出す人が出てきます。

彼らの言い分は、『そもそも、多くのこと(愛や健康や成功)を期待しなければ、手に入らなくても落ちこむことはない。逆に、良い事が起きたら期待していなかったぶんだけハッピーな気分になれる』というものです。しかし、ターリ・シャーロットはこの理論は間違っていると言います。その理由は3つあります。

  1. 結果がどうであれ、成功しても失敗しても期待が高い人の方が常に幸福度は高い
  2. 結果の良し悪しに関わらず、何かを心待ちにしているだけで人は幸せを感じられる
  3. 楽観主義は将来に対する期待によって世界の見方を変えている

現実主義が悪いものと言っているのではありません。しかし、多くの人は、楽観主義がもたらす期待感によって幸福度を上げているのです。

[PR]知って得する、知らないと損をする!すぐに役立つ相手に合った「伝え方」のコツ!

楽観主義の良いところと悪いところ

ターリ・シャーロットたちが行った実験の結果、楽観主義は成功と関係しているだけでなく 成功を導くということが分かっています。楽観主義には多くの利点がありますが、その最たるものが健康でしょう。

未来が明るいと信じていれば、ストレスと不安は軽減されます。

If we expect the future to be bright, stress and anxiety are reduced.

しかし、楽観主義は良いことばかりではありません。さきほど、西洋における離婚率は40%だといいました。ガンになる確率も平均30%で、自分の子供が天才になる可能性も正直まちまちです。ですが、楽観主義の人たちはこれらの数字が自分には当てはまらないと思っているのです。

平均はあくまで平均で、誰にでも起こりうることです。しかし、『楽観主義バイアス』がかかってい人は、それらは自分ではない他人に起こることで、自分には関係ないことだと考えています。逆に、当たらないと言われているのに、宝くじを買うのも『楽観主義バイアス』がかかっている証拠といえます。

楽観主義により、気分が明るくなり健康的にはなります。しかし、その楽観主義によって悪い現実を過小評価し、良い現実を過大評価してしまうのもまた事実なのです。

[PR]しちだの魔法ペンなら35日でバイリンガルに!楽天4部門1位の英会話!<七田式>

『知識』は楽観主義の危険から自分を守ることが出来る

さまざまな統計が出ているにもかかわらず、それを自分の事だと考えないのが『楽観主義バイアス』の短所です。しかし、『楽観主義バイアス』によって、私たちは日々の幸福感を感じることが出来ています。ですので、一概に『楽観主義バイアス』は悪いものではないのです。

私たちが本当にしたいのは、楽観主義の危険から自分たちを守ることです。 しかし同時に楽天的であり続け、楽観主義の数多くの恩恵を享受したいのです。

実現する方法があると考えています。重要なのは知識です。

So what we would really like to do, is we would like to protect ourselves from the dangers of optimism, but at the same time remain hopeful, benefiting from the many fruits of optimism.

And I believe there’s a way for us to do that. The key here really is knowledge.

不健康な生活をしていると体に悪い、というのは誰でも知っていることです。そして、タバコをやめて、過度にお酒を飲まず、バランスの良い食生活を心がけて、適度な運動を行えば、健康的になることもわかっています。これが知識です。

これが、平均で起きてしまうことも知識をもって対策を行うことで、自分に起きる可能性を減らすことが出来る一例といえるでしょう。

[PR]検定試験合格者累計140万人!スマホ対応☆国家資格ほか資格取得ならSMART合格対策講座

まとめ:転ばぬ先の杖を持って楽観的に生きよう

楽観主義バイアスを正しく理解することで、私たちが持つ幻想も壊れることは無くなります。視覚の錯覚と同じように、それを理解しても錯覚が消えるわけではないのです。

これは良いことです。なぜなら、対策やルールを策定することによって、ちょうどいいバランスをとる事ができるからです。そうして、非現実的な楽観主義から身を守り、同時に希望を持ち続けることができます。

And this is good because it means we should be able to strike a balance, to come up with plans and rules to protect ourselves from unrealistic optimism, but at the same time remain hopeful.

たとえば、晴れていてもカバンに折りたたみの傘を入れること。これも、転ばぬ先の杖です。一歩先を行く楽観主義で、毎日を明るく楽しく過ごしたいですね。

英語全文

I’m going to talk to you about optimism — or more precisely, the optimism bias. It’s a cognitive illusion that we’ve been studying in my lab for the past few years, and 80 percent of us have it.

<全文を読む>▼クリック▼

It’s our tendency to overestimate our likelihood of experiencing good events in our lives and underestimate our likelihood of experiencing bad events. So we underestimate our likelihood of suffering from cancer, being in a car accident. We overestimate our longevity, our career prospects. In short, we’re more optimistic than realistic, but we are oblivious to the fact.

Take marriage for example. In the Western world, divorce rates are about 40 percent. That means that out of five married couples,two will end up splitting their assets. But when you ask newlyweds about their own likelihood of divorce, they estimate it at zero percent. And even divorce lawyers, who should really know better, hugely underestimate their own likelihood of divorce. So it turns out that optimists are not less likely to divorce, but they are more likely to remarry. In the words of Samuel Johnson, “Remarriage is the triumph of hope over experience.”

So if we’re married, we’re more likely to have kids. And we all think our kids will be especially talented. This, by the way, is my two-year-old nephew, Guy. And I just want to make it absolutely clear that he’s a really bad example of the optimism bias, because he is in fact uniquely talented.

And I’m not alone. Out of four British people,three said that they were optimistic about the future of their own families. That’s 75 percent. But only 30 percent said that they thought families in general are doing better than a few generations ago.

And this is a really important point, because we’re optimistic about ourselves, we’re optimistic about our kids, we’re optimistic about our families, but we’re not so optimistic about the guy sitting next to us, and we’re somewhat pessimistic about the fate of our fellow citizens and the fate of our country. But private optimism about our own personal future remains persistent. And it doesn’t mean that we think things will magically turn out okay, but rather that we have the unique ability to make it so.

Now I’m a scientist, I do experiments. So to show you what I mean, I’m going to do an experiment here with you. So I’m going to give you a list of abilities and characteristics, and I want you to think for each of these abilities where you stand relative to the rest of the population.

The first one is getting along well with others. Who here believes they’re at the bottom 25 percent? Okay, that’s about 10 people out of 1,500. Who believes they’re at the top 25 percent? That’s most of us here. Okay, now do the same for your driving ability. How interesting are you? How attractive are you? How honest are you? And finally, how modest are you?

So most of us put ourselves above average on most of these abilities. Now this is statistically impossible. We can’t all be better than everyone else. (Laughter) But if we believe we’re better than the other guy, well that means that we’re more likely to get that promotion, to remain married, because we’re more social, more interesting.

And it’s a global phenomenon. The optimism bias has been observed in many different countries — in Western cultures, in non-Western cultures, in females and males, in kids, in the elderly. It’s quite widespread.

But the question is, is it good for us? So some people say no. Some people say the secret to happiness is low expectations. I think the logic goes something like this: If we don’t expect greatness, if we don’t expect to find love and be healthy and successful, well we’re not going to be disappointed when these things don’t happen. And if we’re not disappointed when good things don’t happen, and we’re pleasantly surprised when they do, we will be happy.

So it’s a very good theory, but it turns out to be wrong for three reasons. Number one: Whatever happens, whether you succeed or you fail, people with high expectations always feel better. Because how we feel when we get dumped or win employee of the month depends on how we interpret that event.

The psychologists Margaret Marshall and John Brown studied students with high and low expectations. And they found that when people with high expectations succeed, they attribute that success to their own traits. “I’m a genius, therefore I got an A, therefore I’ll get an A again and again in the future.” When they failed, it wasn’t because they were dumb, but because the exam just happened to be unfair. Next time they will do better. People with low expectations do the opposite. So when they failed it was because they were dumb, and when they succeeded it was because the exam just happened to be really easy. Next time reality would catch up with them. So they felt worse.

Number two: Regardless of the outcome, the pure act of anticipation makes us happy. The behavioral economist George Lowenstein asked students in his university to imagine getting a passionate kiss from a celebrity, any celebrity. Then he said, “How much are you willing to pay to get a kiss from a celebrity if the kiss was delivered immediately, in three hours, in 24 hours, in three days, in one year, in 10 years? He found that the students were willing to pay the most not to get a kiss immediately, but to get a kiss in three days. They were willing to pay extra in order to wait. Now they weren’t willing to wait a year or 10 years; no one wants an aging celebrity. But three days seemed to be the optimum amount.

So why is that? Well if you get the kiss now, it’s over and done with. But if you get the kiss in three days, well that’s three days of jittery anticipation, the thrill of the wait. The students wanted that time to imagine where is it going to happen, how is it going to happen. Anticipation made them happy.

This is, by the way, why people prefer Friday to Sunday. It’s a really curious fact, because Friday is a day of work and Sunday is a day of pleasure, so you’d assume that people will prefer Sunday, but they don’t. It’s not because they really, really like being in the office and they can’t stand strolling in the park or having a lazy brunch. We know that, because when you ask people about their ultimate favorite day of the week, surprise, surprise, Saturday comes in at first, then Friday, then Sunday. People prefer Friday because Friday brings with it the anticipation of the weekend ahead, all the plans that you have. On Sunday, the only thing you can look forward to is the work week.

So optimists are people who expect more kisses in their future, more strolls in the park. And that anticipation enhances their wellbeing. In fact, without the optimism bias, we would all be slightly depressed. People with mild depression, they don’t have a bias when they look into the future. They’re actually more realistic than healthy individuals. But individuals with severe depression, they have a pessimistic bias. So they tend to expect the future to be worse than it ends up being.

So optimism changes subjective reality. The way we expect the world to be changes the way we see it. But it also changes objective reality. It acts as a self-fulfilling prophecy. And that is the third reason why lowering your expectations will not make you happy. Controlled experiments have shown that optimism is not only related to success, it leads to success. Optimism leads to success in academia and sports and politics. And maybe the most surprising benefit of optimism is health. If we expect the future to be bright, stress and anxiety are reduced.

So all in all, optimism has lots of benefits. But the question that was really confusing to me was, how do we maintain optimism in the face of reality? As an neuroscientist, this was especially confusing, because according to all the theories out there, when your expectations are not met, you should alter them. But this is not what we find. We asked people to come into our lab in order to try and figure out what was going on.

We asked them to estimate their likelihood of experiencing different terrible events in their lives. So, for example, what is your likelihood of suffering from cancer? And then we told them the average likelihood of someone like them to suffer these misfortunes. So cancer, for example, is about 30 percent. And then we asked them again, “How likely are you to suffer from cancer?”

What we wanted to know was whether people will take the information that we gave them to change their beliefs. And indeed they did — but mostly when the information we gave them was better than what they expected. So for example, if someone said, “My likelihood of suffering from cancer is about 50 percent,” and we said, “Hey, good news. The average likelihood is only 30 percent,” the next time around they would say, “Well maybe my likelihood is about 35 percent.” So they learned quickly and efficiently. But if someone started off saying, “My average likelihood of suffering from cancer is about 10 percent,” and we said, “Hey, bad news. The average likelihood is about 30 percent,” the next time around they would say, “Yep. Still think it’s about 11 percent.”

So it’s not that they didn’t learn at all — they did — but much, much less than when we gave them positive information about the future. And it’s not that they didn’t remember the numbers that we gave them; everyone remembers that the average likelihood of cancer is about 30 percent and the average likelihood of divorce is about 40 percent. But they didn’t think that those numbers were related to them.

What this means is that warning signs such as these may only have limited impact. Yes, smoking kills, but mostly it kills the other guy.

What I wanted to know was what was going on inside the human brain that prevented us from taking these warning signs personally. But at the same time, when we hear that the housing market is hopeful, we think, “Oh, my house is definitely going to double in price.” To try and figure that out, I asked the participants in the experiment to lie in a brain imaging scanner. It looks like this. And using a method called functional MRI, we were able to identify regions in the brain that were responding to positive information.

One of these regions is called the left inferior frontal gyrus. So if someone said, “My likelihood of suffering from cancer is 50 percent,” and we said, “Hey, good news. Average likelihood is 30 percent,” the left inferior frontal gyrus would respond fiercely. And it didn’t matter if you’re an extreme optimist, a mild optimist or slightly pessimistic, everyone’s left inferior frontal gyrus was functioning perfectly well, whether you’re Barack Obama or Woody Allen.

On the other side of the brain, the right inferior frontal gyrus was responding to bad news. And here’s the thing: it wasn’t doing a very good job. The more optimistic you were, the less likely this region was to respond to unexpected negative information. And if your brain is failing at integrating bad news about the future, you will constantly leave your rose-tinted spectacles on.

So we wanted to know, could we change this? Could we alter people’s optimism bias by interfering with the brain activity in these regions? And there’s a way for us to do that.

This is my collaborator Ryota Kanai. And what he’s doing is he’s passing a small magnetic pulse through the skull of the participant in our study into their inferior frontal gyrus. And by doing that, he’s interfering with the activity of this brain region for about half an hour. After that everything goes back to normal, I assure you.

So let’s see what happens. First of all, I’m going to show you the average amount of bias that we see. So if I was to test all of you now, this is the amount that you would learn more from good news relative to bad news. Now we interfere with the region that we found to integrate negative information in this task, and the optimism bias grew even larger. We made people more biased in the way that they process information. Then we interfered with the brain region that we found to integrate good news in this task, and the optimism bias disappeared. We were quite amazed by these results because we were able to eliminate a deep-rooted bias in humans.

And at this point we stopped and we asked ourselves, would we want to shatter the optimism illusion into tiny little bits? If we could do that, would we want to take people’s optimism bias away? Well I’ve already told you about all of the benefits of the optimism bias, which probably makes you want to hold onto it for dear life. But there are, of course, pitfalls, and it would be really foolish of us to ignore them.

Take for example this email I recieved from a firefighter here in California. He says, “Fatality investigations for firefighters often include ‘We didn’t think the fire was going to do that,’ even when all of the available information was there to make safe decisions.” This captain is going to use our findings on the optimism bias to try to explain to the firefighters why they think the way they do, to make them acutely aware of this very optimistic bias in humans.

So unrealistic optimism can lead to risky behavior, to financial collapse, to faulty planning. The British government, for example, has acknowledged that the optimism bias can make individuals more likely to underestimate the costs and durations of projects. So they have adjusted the 2012 Olympic budget for the optimism bias.

My friend who’s getting married in a few weeks has done the same for his wedding budget. And by the way, when I asked him about his own likelihood of divorce, he said he was quite sure it was zero percent.

So what we would really like to do, is we would like to protect ourselves from the dangers of optimism, but at the same time remain hopeful, benefiting from the many fruits of optimism. And I believe there’s a way for us to do that. The key here really is knowledge. We’re not born with an innate understanding of our biases. These have to be identified by scientific investigation. But the good news is that becoming aware of the optimism bias does not shatter the illusion. It’s like visual illusions, in which understanding them does not make them go away. And this is good because it means we should be able to strike a balance, to come up with plans and rules to protect ourselves from unrealistic optimism, but at the same time remain hopeful.

I think this cartoon portrays it nicely. Because if you’re one of these pessimistic penguins up there who just does not believe they can fly, you certainly never will. Because to make any kind of progress, we need to be able to imagine a different reality, and then we need to believe that that reality is possible. But if you are an extreme optimistic penguin who just jumps down blindly hoping for the best, you might find yourself in a bit of a mess when you hit the ground. But if you’re an optimistic penguin who believes they can fly, but then adjusts a parachute to your back just in case things don’t work out exactly as you had planned, you will soar like an eagle, even if you’re just a penguin.

Thank you.

<閉じる>

\ ほかにも気になるトークが満載! /

マイク
TEDまとめ(1):エキスパートたちが贈る極上のメッセージ ライフハックとしてではなく、英語学習にも極めて有用なのが、著名人が10分程度のプレゼンを行うTEDです。 TED Talksと...